Clear48° FULL FORECASTClear48° FULL FORECAST
Make us your home page
Instagram

Balloon boy saga reveals the complexity of translating emotions

Richard Heene showed what seemed to be genuine emotion when 6-year-old Falcon was found. But an expert says a shrug of one shoulder at a press conference suggested Heene had lied.

Associated Press

Richard Heene showed what seemed to be genuine emotion when 6-year-old Falcon was found. But an expert says a shrug of one shoulder at a press conference suggested Heene had lied.

Shortly after authorities announced that 6-old-year Falcon Heene had been found safe and sound, ending speculation that he had been aboard a flying saucer that escaped from his family's back yard, his father, Richard, appeared before an encampment of cameras to share a few words of relief.

"He says he was hiding in the attic," Heene said, his voice swelling on the last two syllables as he half-shrugged and looked at the ground. "And, um, because I yelled at him." He took a sharp breath, voice faltering. "I'm really sorry I yelled at him."

That night, CNN's Jane Velez-Mitchell described a "very emotional news conference just moments ago, the dad clearly choking up." The next morning, Larimer County Sheriff Jim Alderden told the network that "it was real obvious from (the Heenes') demeanor early on that they thought he was in the balloon." Later that day, he told Wolf Blitzer, "I can tell you, having got to the scene somewhat after the boy came out, I — I, too, saw the parents were visibly shaken by this event and certainly seemed very credible." Mayumi Heene's 911 call, released the next day, was even more convincing: This woman was obviously distraught.

Is there ever anything to be learned from the way a suspect reacts at the scene of the crime? We now know, of course, that Richard and Mayumi Heene were merely credible actors who duped the networks and the sheriff. This was a failure of what we'll call emotional forensics: the process of determining whether someone's reaction to a crisis is genuine. It's not just an academic question. As the New Yorker's David Grann recounted in his recent examination of Cameron Todd Willingham, the man executed in Texas for an arson he almost certainly didn't commit, Willingham's reaction at the scene of the fire was alternately presented as evidence for and against him. A police chaplain at the scene originally described Willingham as being so hysterical that he needed to be physically restrained. As other evidence turned against him, however, a neighbor reported that Willingham "did not appear to be excited or concerned" — a claim the chaplain would later amend his story to include.

There is little empirical study of how well people can detect a feigned emotion, but there are many studies of how well they can detect liars. Experts examine the way a person who's fibbing contorts and shifts his face in a way that reveals hidden emotions, like delight at having duped someone. In the end, though, lie detection is about guessing whether a particular statement, or set of statements, is true. By contrast, emotional forensics would involve judging the veracity of the emotion itself based on how the subject behaves.

The field of lie detection can tell us that, in many cases, a person's inability to cloak his real emotions during a performance will tip his hand. Lying studies titan Paul Ekman describes this process as "leakage."

Very few people can entirely mask the real emotions they're feeling, nor can they fake all the facial cues of an emotion they're not feeling in just the right sequence. When I sent Ekman the video of Heene's makeshift press conference, he wrote back: "He shows one of our most reliable signs of lying, which is (a) FRAGMENT of a shrug, just once, just one shoulder, not definitive but consistent with his lying."

Detecting fake emotions may be more difficult than identifying a lie. For one thing, facial expressions and body language tend to be remarkably consistent across people and cultures — Ekman discovered, early in his career, that even tribesmen in Papua, New Guinea, with little exposure to outside culture interpreted expressions the same way the rest of us do. But our manners of displaying emotion are not nearly as universal. We may all bunch our eyebrows when afraid, but we do not all display the same gross behavior when we're terrified.

University of San Francisco psychologist Maureen O'Sullivan brought up the case of Lindy Chamberlain, the Australian woman who claimed she saw a dingo run off with her infant daughter during a camping trip in 1980. Chamberlain's stoic response when questioned about the case convinced many that she had murdered her child. Her response did not square with their sense of how a person in that situation should react. Chamberlain was found guilty of the crime and spent several years in prison before the infant's jacket was discovered, corroborating her version of events.

The trouble with situations like the Heene fiasco or the Willingham case is that most people consider themselves very accurate judges of emotion. We all know from personal experience that it's fairly easy to pass off a factual lie, so we don't expect to be able to tell when others are doing the same. But judging someone's emotional performance seems far more instinctual, and we tend to assume that we're good at it. This may be correct — very few people are able to feign emotion with any degree of verisimilitude. The ones who can do it often earn a very handsome living.

I don't know whether Richard Heene is up on his Stanislavsky, but we know from his Wife Swap appearances that he is a devoted thespian who shows no apparent remorse at manipulating his family or the public in the pursuit of fame. This works in his favor: The fewer conflicting emotions you have behind the one you're trying to pull off, the less likely you are to "leak" emotions and expose yourself as a fraud. In any case, his acting success on the national stage suggests that we might do well to avoid using a suspect's emotional response as evidence one way or another.

Balloon boy saga reveals the complexity of translating emotions 10/31/09 [Last modified: Saturday, October 31, 2009 4:31am]
Photo reprints | Article reprints

© 2014 Tampa Bay Times

    

Join the discussion: Click to view comments, add yours

Loading...